What is mate poaching?

February 8, 2024.

What is mate poaching?

When it comes to romance, some folks see it as a competition. If they set their sights on someone, they will openly compete to entice them away from their current relationship.

  • The term is “mate poaching” (Schmitt et al. 2004; Schmitt and Buss 2001). Although both genders engage in mate poaching, I will be focusing on female mate poachers because it is, by far, the most prevalent form of mate poaching we discuss in couples therapy.

  • In a research study conducted in 2004, 54% of the women “poached” their current partner out of a previous relationship.

As a high-end couples therapist working with “C” class executives, I see this all the time.

But it’s awkward and not at all PC to discuss.

Researchers in the United States and Australia are learning more about how these relationships form and how they differ from relationships formed by those previously unattached. Evolutionary psychologists are even more concerned about the behavior of mate poaching.

Research tells us that mate poaching is widespread and found across the globe. It is a common interpersonal issue in most countries and cultures. According to an extensive survey of over 16,000 people worldwide, evolutionary psychologist David Schmitt believes that mate poaching impacts as many as 10 to 15% of all committed relationships.

Mate poaching has always been with us to some degree.

I am an avid fan of evolutionary psychologist David M. Buss. Every couples therapist should be familiar with David’s work. He tells us that women have always competed against each other for the attention of Alpha males.

Female mate poaching tactics are a common strategy…

In societies where desirable men are hard to find, women will aggressively compete for them by poaching. Forget notions of sisterhood. Suzy Welch is your new role model.

Another reason why mate poaching is so universal is that it’s a relatively safe strategy for women with little danger to its downside. Men directly run the risk of violence in mate poaching.

  • But lethal force between female competitors is relatively rare. Mate-poaching isn’t unique to humans. It has been observed among various species (Dawkins and Krebs 1978; Dewaal 1986; Trivers 1985) and across diverse human cultural groups (e.g., Schmitt, et al. 2004).

Mate poaching can be dramatic, risky, and emotional.

  • Stealing someone else’s spouse can be both difficult and dangerous (see Davies, Shackleford, and Hass, 2010; Schmitt and Buss, 2001).

The odds of being rejected are higher than average, mainly if the targeted potential mate is married or otherwise profoundly committed to their current relationship (Schmitt and Buss (2001).

Another problem is that mate-poaching is a high-drama endeavor…

Strong feelings are in play. Mate-poachers and their targets may feel high levels of stress or guilt (Davies, et al. 2010; Schmitt and Buss, 2001).

Mate poaching may result in acts of retaliation from the cuckold defending their turf or social disapproval (Daly and Wilson, 1989).

Even if they succeed in temporarily capturing interest, the risk of being dumped is high. No wonder why mate-poaching tactics are sly, sneaky, and subtle. How do mate poachers operate?

How does a mate poacher set a trap? It typically starts with a covetous lingering gaze. Other tactics may include flirtation, provocative body language, or spying on the relationship through a shared to probe for signs of weakness or dissatisfaction that can be exploited (Schmitt and Buss 2001). Mate poachers often draw attention to themselves by asking for help with a work-related question.

  • Then they gradually build a Platonic friendship with their intended target. It’s typical for mate poachers to shift the conversational content over time. Asking increasingly intimate and exploratory questions is a common strategy for the workplace mate poacher (Mogilski & Wade, 2013).

Many mate poachers are themselves often already involved in a relationship.

They will manipulate or deceive their partner and act in unusual ways. For example, researchers tell us that mate poachers who are already in committed relationships might act even more affectionately with their current partner to hide their efforts to poach another partner and avoid suspicion.

Mate poachers and the problem of permissive environments

  • Research suggests that many mate poachers are high on a set of personality traits called the Dark Triad. These are traits of Machiavellian intelligence, narcissism, and psychopathy (Jonason, Li, & Buss, 2010). Workplace mate poachers have inherently unstable intimate relationships. They have trouble holding on to partners and are often freely willing to be poached away.

  • We also now know that Mate Poachers are rated as better-looking than non-poachers (Sunderani, Arnocky, & Vaillancourt, 2013). They often present with style and allure. This makes sense, as their reproductive strategy is to entice a high-value partner into a relationship.

  • Some mate poachers are openly provocative flirts, but many are more devious. Mate poachers and their hunting grounds Josephs (2016) tells us that infidelity cannot occur without a “permissive environment.” A permissive environment is maintained by co-workers willing to have sex with you although you (or they) are in a committed relationship.

  • The more people continue to work in co-ed settings, the more the mate poacher can assess the available inventory of seducible partners. Josephs (2016) reports that individual psychotherapy with mate poachers is challenging because the drama of the high risk/high reward brings the therapist along on a very bumpy ride.

As a reproductive strategy, mate poaching has the dopamine hit of the massive ego boost you get by slyly winning a highly desirable partner…or the sudden crash of becoming the cast-off third wheel.

Neuroscience tells us this is the same neural reward-seeking circuitry found in problem gamblers (Joseph, 2016). It may be addictive to be a mate poacher.

Mate poachers in therapy

It’s a fascinating phenomenon that mate poachers often enter individual therapy when they feel that the romantic tide is turning against them.

It is a puzzle of neuroscience that mate poachers are often unable or unwilling to tolerate defeat. Researchers are still wrestling with the question.

Why would someone be committed to winning a fundamentally untrustworthy partner when honesty and trustworthiness are much preferred, desirable traits in a long-term partner?”

Is it because the mate poacher is addicted to the pursuit? The mate poacher is ever hopeful for a desirable but spoken-for partner…

They are perpetually looking to trade up. Or perhaps a mate poacher is in a bad relationship and cushions the blow with a workplace exit affair. Mate poachers in therapy can present along with a moral continuum. This is why therapists recruit such continuums at their peril.

Who could blame a woman in an abusive relationship for being attracted to her kind, stable, but unhappily married boss?

And yet at the same time, research suggests that a significant number of mate poachers have personality disorders such as narcissism or Borderline Personality Disorder (Jonason, Li, & Buss, 2010). Mate poachers and masculine privilege Successful men often perceive that they are entitled to enjoy the fruits of their labor by taking advantage of the women who are enamored of them.

However, the # MeToo movement has brought about a cultural shift…

Employers are re-evaluating how “permissive” their work culture should be.

Most notable is the fact that unattached CEOs are being fired for engaging in even consensual relationships with mate poachers.

The common stereotype of the powerful man and the trophy woman is consistent with research findings across cultures and across time that men are attracted to beauty and youth, and women are attracted to power and resources. Research has sadly proven this cliche to be true.

“These prominent men are captivated by opportunities for the conquest of beauty queens who scorned them when they were young, awkward, and starting their climb up the ladder. What they usually fail to recognize or consider is that they become a trophy themselves when they take the giant step down from their lofty pedestal to engage in an illicit sexual relationship.” Dr. Shirley Glass.

Mate poaching and the enlightened HR department…

It would be helpful for HR Departments to study clinical research on mate poaching. The HR problem with mate poachers and their prey in a corporate setting is the serial social havoc they incur. We know that mate poachers have an aversion to cutting their losses when things get messy.

That’s why mate poachers have been known to serially harass and cyber-stalk their affair partner’s wives, while HR departments wring their hands helplessly on the sidelines. Cultural blinders are fascinating. We are living in a time of revelation and disclosure. HR departments have perfected sexual harassment training to a meme. The bad behavior of men is on display as never before.

The floodgates of reproach have been thrown open. Many mighty men will be crushed beneath the wheel of this cultural shift. This sometimes happens when consciousness expands. So be it.

We need to be less gyno-centric. Hopefully, at some point, our consciousness might expand once more to take in the phenomena of workplace mate poaching.

HR departments are sorely in need of comprehensive training to foster a family-friendly workplace culture with appropriate boundaries.

Thanks to the late Dr. Shirley Glass, there is preventive, teachable, evidence-based research that could inform an HR workplace initiative mate poaching prevention program.

A permissive work environment is not just created by a mate poacher…

Corporate and workplace culture have a role to play as well.

“Secluded from the responsibilities of everyday life, the parallel universe of the affair is often idealized, infused with the promise of transcendence. For some people…it is a world of possibility—an alternate reality in which they can reimagine and reinvent themselves. Then again, it is experienced as limitless precisely because it is contained within the limits of its clandestine structure. It is a poetic interlude in a prosaic life.” Esther Perel.

Mate poachers thrive in permissive environments, which is why workplace culture has become increasingly on the radar. Many mate poachers prefer to transition from one relationship by seamlessly “trading-up” to another, and sometimes the best place to do this is at work.

  • The Mate-Switching Hypothesis This “trading-up” behavior has been called the Mate-Switching Hypothesis (Greiling and Buss, 2000). “Trading-up” means the potential new partner is a higher-quality mate than a current partner.

  • Although “sperm of the moment relationships” are easy to find, most women want to see and maintain long-term committed relationships, and mate poachers are willing to look at all available options and compete for who they want.

  • According to Schmitt (2004), 44% of women in his study reportedly engaged in mate-poach to find a committed, long-term relationship.

  • If the notion of spouse poaching morally unencumbers you, research tells us that your choice of a high-status man will result in more healthy children than you might have with a man of lower status (Bereczkei and Csansky 1996; Voland and Engel 1990).

Sexual Attractiveness

  • Let’s talk about sexual attractiveness. Evolutionary biologists claim it is a placeholder for higher sperm quality and fertility (Gallup and Frederick, 2010). Attractive partners…attract.

  • Mate poachers are more likely to be desired by others (Buss, 1989; Overbeek and Engels, 2010). Some researchers believe that mate poachers prefer to rely on the tastes and preferences of other women. This idea is called Mate-Choice Copying (Graziiano et al. 1993; Parker and Burkley, 2009). Waynforth (2007).

These researchers suggest that mate poachers consider men in existing relationships as more desirable just because another woman already selected them. Does mate poaching work?

Women typically employ the cultural expectation that women are more reliably faithful than men. The “other woman” is often attacked as being a “homewrecker,” and a threat to community standards. However, some experts believe our views on marriage are changing.

Community standards are sometimes only the lies we pretend to abide by. Although it has been declining over the past few decades, divorce in the U.S. is still robust enough to assume that a reasonable number of desirable men will become available again at some point in the future.

Infidelity is not only as common as it ever was, it now reflects a level of emotional intensity unseen in prior decades.

Do affairs that result from aggressive mate poaching lead to marriage?

“Slut-shaming” isn’t the reliable defense it once was. Relationships are complicated and shift over time. The cultural expectation that marriage is a “forever commitment” is wobbly at best. Extra-marital liaisons are losing their capacity to shock and offend.

Cultural shifts matter.

In a recent magazine poll, 57% of women reported that they would respect a female friend to a lesser degree if they knew that she was with a married man. 77% of women would think less of a male friend involved in an affair. While this indicates somewhat high rates of discomfort, the absolute social norm condemning “adultery” a century ago has been seriously undermined.

However, spouse poaching is still a riskier move than mate poaching. In some states, it’s even against the law. Real success often eludes the mate poacher The important takeaway is that while men are reporting a higher level of emotional involvement during the affair, research continues to indicate that after divorce, unions with affair partners are relatively rare and risky to the extreme.

A recent poll of 4,126 male business executives discovered that only 3% of those divorced did so because they became committed to their affair partners. And 86% of male respondents to a recent magazine poll reported that they passed on their affair partner as their next life mate.

If you are a member of the small group that does end up married to your affair partner, you still have not beaten the odds…

The divorce rate for these couples within the first 5 years of marriage is 70%.

The math doesn’t lie.

Post-divorce bliss with an affair partner is highly unlikely.

Mate poaching as a reliable, long-term life partner-securing strategy for women in the 21st century is dicey at best.

Final Thoughts

  • Researchers Davies, Shackleford, and Hass (2007) conclude that most men and women would prefer to pair bond with someone unattached.

Mate poaching is typically a “Plan B” when all else fails. It’s risky, high drama and success often comes at a high social cost.

But Mate-poaching is everywhere. It’s so pervasive because, for some humans, it works pretty well some of the time. The amoral mate poacher may have to kiss a few frogs and get their reputation dinged a bit, and yet mate poaching continues to interfere with marriages and committed relationships worldwide.

Be well, stay kind and Godspeed.

RESEARCH:

ARNOCKY, S., SUNDERANI, S., MILLER, J. and VAILLANCOURT, T. (2012): Jealousy mediates the relationship between attractiveness comparison and females’ indirect aggression. Personal Relationships, 19(2), 290–303. DOI:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01362.x

BAKER, R. R., and BELLIS, M. A. (1995): Human Sperm Competition: Copulation, masturbation, and infidelity. London: Chapman and Hall.

BAUMEISTER, R. F., CATANESE, K. R. and VOHS, K. D. (2001): Are there gender differences in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, conceptual distinctions, and a review of relevant evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(3), 242–73. DOI: 10.1207/ S15327957PSPR0503_5

BENSHOOF, L. and THORNHILL, R. (1979): The evolution of monogamy and loss of estrus in humans. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 2, 95–106.

BERECZKEI, T. and CSANAKY, A. (1996): Evolutionary pathway of child development: Lifestyles of adolescents and adults from father-absent families. Human Nature, 7, 257–280. DOI: 10.1007/BF02733397

BERSCHEID, E., DION, K., WALSTER, E. and WALSTER, G. W. (1971): Physical attractiveness and dating choice: A test of the matching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 173–189.

BROUDE, G. J., and GREENE, S. J. (1976): Cross-cultural codes on twenty sexual attitudes and practices. Ethnology, 15, 409–438. BUSS, D. M. (2007): The evolution of human mating. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 39(3), 502–512.

STEVEN ARNOCKY, SHAFIK SUNDERANI, TRACY VAILLANCOURT JEP 11(2013)2 80 BUSS, D. M. (2006): Strategies of human mating. Psychological Topics, 15, 239–260.

BUSS, D. M. (2004): Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind. Boston, MA: Pearson.

BUSS, D. M. (2000): The Dangerous Passion: Why Jealousy is as Necessary as Love and Sex. New York: Free Press. BUSS, D. M. (1994): The strategies of human mating. American Scientist, 82, 238–94.

BUSS, D. M. (1989): Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1–49.

BUSS, D. M. and SCHMITT, D. P. (1993): Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100(2), 204–232. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.2.204

BUUNK, A. P., and FISHER, M. (2009): Individual differences in intrasexual competition. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 37–48. DOI: 10.1556/JEP.7.2009.1.5

DAVIES, A. P. C., SHACKELFORD, T. K. and GOETZ, A. T. (2006a): An evolutionary psychological perspective on infidelity. In P. R. Peluso (Ed.), In Love’s Debris: A Practitioner’s Guide to Addressing Infidelity in Couples Therapy (Routledge series on Family Therapy). New York: Routledge.

DAVIES, A. P., SHACKELFORD, T. K., and GOETZ, A. T. (2006b): “Attached” or “Unattached”: With who do men and women prefer to mate, and why? Psychological Topics, 15(2), 297–314. DAVIES, A. P., SHACKELFORD, T. K. and HASS, R. G. (2010): Sex differences in the perceptions of benefits and costs of mate poaching. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 441– 445. DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2010.04.014

DAVIES, A. P., SHACKELFORD, T. K. and HASS, R. G. (2007): When a poach is not a poach: Redefining human mate poaching and re-estimating its frequency. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 702–716. DOI: 10.1007/s10508-006-9158-8

DAWKINS, R. and KREBS, J. R. (1978): Animal signals: Information or manipulation. In J. R. Krebs and N. B. Davies (Eds). Behavioral Ecology (pp. 282–309). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific.

DEWAAL, F. (1986): Deception in the natural communication of chimpanzees. In R. W. Mitchell and N. S. Thompson (Eds), Deception (pp. 221–244). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. ELLIS, B. J. (1992): The evolution of sexual attraction: Evaluative mechanisms in women. In L. Cosmides, J. Tooby, and J. Tooby (Eds). The Adapted Mind (pp. 195–231). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

FIGUEREDO, A. J., SEFCEK, J., and JONES, D. N. (2006): The ideal romantic partner personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 431–441. DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2006.02.004

GALLUP, G. and FREDERICK, D. A. (2010): The science of sex appeal: An evolutionary perspective. Review of General Psychology, 14, 240–250. DOI:10.1037/a0020451

GANGESTAD, S. W., and THORNHILL, R. (1997): The evolutionary psychology of extrapair sex: the role of fluctuating asymmetry. Evolution and Human Behavior, 8, 69–88.

GOETZ, A. T., SHACKELFORD, T. K., WEEKES-SHACKELFORD, V. A., EULER, H. A., HOIER, S., SCHMITT, D. P. and LAMUNYON, C. W. (2005): Mate retention, semen displacement, and human sperm competition: A preliminary investigation of tactics to prevent and correct female infidelity. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 749–763. DOI: 10.1016/j.paid. 2004.05.028

GOTTMAN, J. (1999): The Marriage Clinic: A Scientifically Based Marital Therapy. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.

GRAZIANO, W. G., JENSEN-CAMPBELL, L. A., SHEBILSKE, L. J. and LUNDGRE, S. R. (1993): Social influence, sex differences, and judgments of beauty: Putting the interpersonal back in interpersonal attraction. Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 522–531. DOI: 10.1037/0022- 3514.65.3.522

GREILING, H. and BUSS, D. M. (2000): Women’s sexual strategies: The hidden dimension of extra-pair mating. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 929–963.

HA, T., OVERBEEK, G., and ENGELS, R. (2010): Effects of attractiveness and social status on dating desire in heterosexual adolescents: An experimental study. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 1063–1071. DOI: 10.1007/s10508-009-9561-z

HARRIS, C. R. (2003): Factors associated with jealousy over real and imagined infidelity: An examination of the social-cognitive and evolutionary psychology perspectives. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27, 319–329. DOI: 10.1111/1471-6402.00112

HILL, K., and HURTANDO, A. M. (1996): Ache Life History: The Ecology and Demography of a Foraging People. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

HODGES-SIMEON, C. R., GAULIN, S. J. C. and PUTS, D. A. (2011): Voice correlates of mating success in men: Examining ‘‘contests” versus ‘‘mate choice” modes of sexual selection. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 551–557. DOI:10.1007/s10508-010-9625-0. J

OKELA, M. (2009): Physical attractiveness and reproductive success in humans: Evidence from the last 20th century United States. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 342–350. DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.03.006

JOKELA, M., ROTKIRCH, A., RICKARD, I. J., PETTAY, J. and LUMMAA, V. (2010): Serial monogamy increases reproductive success in men but not in women. Behavioral Ecology, 21(5), 906– 912. DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arq078

KIRSNER, B. R., FIGUEREDO, A. J. and JACOBS, W. J. (2003): Self, friends, and lovers: structural relations among Beck Depression Inventory scores and perceived mate values. Journal of Affective Disorders, 75, 131–148. DOI: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00048-4

LANCASTER, J. B and LANCASTER, C. (1987): The watershed: Change in parental-investment and family-formation strategies in the course of human evolution. In: Lancaster, J. B, Altmann, J, Rossi, A. S. and Sherrod, L. R., (Eds). Parenting Across the Life Span: Biosocial DimenSions (pp. 187–205). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

MARLOWE, F. (2000): Paternal investment and the human mating system. Behavioral Processes, 51, 45–61.

MILLER, G. F. (2000): The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of Human Nature. New York, NY: Doubleday.

MILLER, L. C., PUTCHA-BHAGAVATULA, A. and PEDERSEN, W. C. (2002): Men’s and women’s mating preferences: Distinct evolutionary mechanisms? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 88–93. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8721.00175

PARKER, J. and BURKLEY, M. (2009): Who’s chasing whom? The impact of gender and relationship status on mate poaching. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1016–1019. DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.04.022

PLACE, S. S., TODD, P. M., LARS, A. and ASENDORPF, J. B. (2010): Humans show mate copying after observing real mate choices. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 320–325. DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.001

STEVEN ARNOCKY, SHAFIK SUNDERANI, TRACY VAILLANCOURT JEP 11(2013)2 82

RHODES, G., SIMMONS, L. W. and PETERS, M. (2005): Attractiveness and sexual behavior: Does attractiveness enhance mating success? Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 186–201. DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.014

SCHMITT, D. P. (2002): A meta-analysis of sex differences in romantic attraction: Do rating contexts moderate tactic effectiveness judgements? British Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 387–402. DOI: 10.1348/014466602760344278 S

SCHMITT, D. P., et al. (2004): Patterns and universals of mate poaching across 53 nations: The effects of sex, culture, and personality on romantically attracting another person’s partner. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(4), 560–584. DOI: 10.1037/0022- 3514.86.4.560

SCHMITT, D. P., and BUSS, D. M. (2001): Human mate poaching: Tactics and temptations for infiltrating existing mateships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 894–917. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.894

SCHMITT, D. P., and SHACKELFORD, T. (2003): Nifty ways to leave your lover: The tactics people use to entice and disguise the process of human mate poaching. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(8), 1018–1035. DOI: 10.1177/0146167203253471

SCHMITT, D. P., SHACKELFORD, T., DUNTLEY, J., TOOKE, W. and BUSS, D. M. (2001): The desire for sexual variety as a tool for understanding basic human mating strategies. Personal Relationships, 8, 425–455. DOI: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2001.tb00049.x

SHACKELFORD, T. K., GOETZ, A. T., BUSS, D. M., EULER, H. A. and HOIER, S. (2005): When we hurt the ones we love: Predicting violence against women from men’s mate retention tactics. Personal Relationships, 12, 447–463. DOI: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2005.00125.x

SHACKELFORD, T. K., POUND, N., and GOETZ, A. T. (2005): Psychological and physiological adaptation to human sperm competition. Review of General Psychology, 9, 228–248. DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.9.3.228

SPRECHER, S. and HATFIELD, E. (2009): Matching hypothesis. In H. Reis and S. Sprecher (Eds) Encyclopedia of Human Relationships. New York: SAGE. SUNDERANI, S.,

ARNOCKY, S. and VAILLANCOURT, T. (2013): Individual differences in mate poaching: An examination of hormonal, dispositional, and behavioral mate-value traits. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42(4), 533–542. DOI: 10.1007/s10508-012-9974-y

ULLER, T. and JOHANSSON, L. C. (2003): Human mate choice and the wedding ring effect: Are married men more attractive? Human Nature, 14, 267–276. DOI: 10.1007/s12110-003- 1006-0

VOLAND, E. and ENGEL, C. (1990): Female choice in humans: A conditional mate selection strategy of the Krummhorn Women (Germany, 1720–1874). Ethology, 84, 144–154. DOI: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1990.tb00791.x VORACEK, M. (2005): Shortcomings of the sociosexual orientation inventory: Can psychometrics inform evolutionary psychology? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 296–297.

WALSTER, E, ARONSON, J, ABRAHAMS, D, and ROTTMAN, L. (1966): Importance of physical attractiveness in dating behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4(5), 508–516. MATE-POACHING AND MATING SUCCESS IN HUMANS JEP 11(2013)2 83

WASHBURN, S. L., and LANCASTER, C. S. (1966): The evolution of human hunting. In: Lee, R. and DeVore, L, (Eds). Man the Hunter (pp 293–320). Chicago: Aldine de Gruyter.

WAYNFORTH, D. (2007): Mate choice copying in humans. Human Nature, 18, 264–271. DOI: 10.1007/s12110-007-9004-2

WEEDEN, J. and SABINI, J. (2007): Subjective and objective measures of attractiveness and their relation to sexual behavior and sexual attitudes in university students. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 79–88. DOI: 10.1007/s10508-006-9075-x

Previous
Previous

Personality change after heart surgery

Next
Next

Aubiographical Memory and Depression…